Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Fluids and Barriers of the CNS 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research

Serial Tap Test of patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus: impact on cognitive function and its meaning

verfasst von: Samanta Fabrício Blattes da Rocha, Pedro André Kowacs, Ricardo Krause Martinez de Souza, Matheus Kahakura Franco Pedro, Ricardo Ramina, Hélio A. Ghizoni Teive

Erschienen in: Fluids and Barriers of the CNS | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus (INPH) is characterized by gait disturbance, urinary incontinence and cognitive decline. Symptoms are potentially reversible and treatment is based on cerebrospinal fluid shunting. The tap test (TT) is used to identify patients that will benefit from surgery. This procedure consists of the withdrawal of 20 to 50 mL of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) through a lumbar puncture (LP) after which the symptoms of the triad are tested. Improvement in the quality and speed of gait are already recognized but cognitive improvement depends on several factors such as tests used, the time elapsed after LP for re-testing, and the number of punctures. Serial punctures may trigger similar conditions as external lumbar drainage (ELD) to the organism.

Objective

This study aimed to identify how serial punctures affect cognition to increase the sensitivity of the test and consequently the accuracy of surgical indication.

Methods

Sixty-one patients with INPH underwent baseline memory and executive tests repeatedly following the 2-Step Tap Test protocol (2-STT – two procedures of 30 mL lumbar CSF drainage separated by a 24-h interval). The baseline scores of INPH patients were compared with those of 55 healthy controls, and with intragroup post-puncture scores of the 2-STT.

Results

The group with INPH had lower performance than the control group in all cognitive tests (RAVLT, Stroop, CFT, FAR-COWA, FAB, MMSE, orientation, mental control), except for the forward digit span test (p = 0.707). After conducting LP procedures, the Stroop test (words, colors and errors), RAVLT (stage A1, A6 and B1), and CFT (immediate and delayed R) scores were equal to those of the control group (p > 0.05). The INPH group presented significant improvement after the first puncture in MMSE (p = 0.031) and in the Stroop Test (points) (p < 0.001). After the second puncture, subjects improved in orientation, MMSE, RAVLT (B1), Stroop (points, words, errors) and CFT (IR).

Conclusion

Progressive cognitive improvement occurred over the 2-STT and changes were more significant after the second LP in all cognitive domains except for RAVLT (A7). Encephalic alert system ‘arousal’ seems to participate in early improvements observed during 2-STT. The second LP increased the sensitivity of the drainage test to detect changes in cognitive variables, and consequently improved the quality of the method.
Hinweise

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus (INPH) is characterized by the classic triad [1] of progressive symptoms: gait apraxia, dementia, and urinary incontinence resulting from reasons that are not fully explained [2]. Cognitive alterations involve executive, attention, memory, and processing speed dysfunctions [3].
Symptoms of INPH can be alleviated with ventriculoperitoneal shunts [4]. To emulate this procedure, tests that promote the temporary removal of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) are used to evaluate effects on gait and cognition. The most used methods are: a) the lumbar drainage test, known as Tap Test (TT); and b) the continuous lumbar drainage test (CLD). TT has high specificity (73–100%) but low sensitivity (26–79%) [5]. Marmarou et al. [5] and Ishikawa et al. [6] confirmed CLD to be more sensitive (50–100%) than TT, but it is less popular due to invasiveness and morbidity [7, 8].
Aspects such as the amount of CSF drained and measurements of changes in gait and cognition may influence TT outcomes. There is evidence that the effects of CSF drainage in TT can extend beyond the time needed for CSF to regain its original volume and can last for 24 h or longer [9]. Despite the well-established methods to quantify gait improvement, there are still controversies about cognition improvement with TT [3].
Although memory mechanisms as learning, retention and retrieval are well known [10, 11], research on memory has not fully clarified the mechanisms of forgetting. The theory of interference [1214] postulates that forgetfulness derives from the interference of one memory over another. There is evidence that the most important mechanisms are proactive interference (PI), in which prior learning affects later learning, and retroactive interference (RI), in which new information may interfere with prior learning. Thus, given the multiple mnemonic systems that are interacting mutually, studies using complex tests such as the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) may help us to understand how this interaction occurs in a clinical condition that affects memory as INPH.
Ishikawa et al. [6] suggest that cognitive and urinary improvements may still occur up to one week after lumbar puncture. Serial lumbar punctures can produce, by analogy, similar physiological effects to CLD, with corresponding effects on gait and cognition. Thus, with multiple punctures, it may be possible that potential later cognitive amelioration can emerge earlier. The aim of present study was to evaluate the cognitive impact of serial tap test (STT) in patients with INPH.

Material and methods

The protocol was approved by the local regulatory committee for having followed the principles of the Helsinki Convention and its later amendments, as well as Brazilian guidelines disposed by resolution No. 466/2012, all individuals were adequately consented.

Population

INPH population

Study subjects were initially searched at the CSF Circulation Disturbance Research Program 2004–2017 files. This database contains information on well-established standardized procedures carried out at the Neurological Institute of Curitiba in suspected INPH subjects. Of the hundred and forty-eight subjects suspected of having INPH initially found, ten were not included for being aged 58 years or less, 27 for being classified as “unlikely”, three for having been previously submitted to a neurosurgical procedure and further twelve for refusing to consent with the study procedures and other reasons (e.g.: missing data). Of the remaining 96 individuals with "probable" or "possible" INPH (according to the 2005 Euro-American Consensus) [5] were selected. Of those 96 subjects, 30 individuals were excluded due to comorbidities that could potentially impact cognitive functioning, such as active alcoholism, Parkinson's disease or parkinsonism, Alzheimer's disease, epilepsy, central nervous system [CNS] tumor, other neurological diseases such as vascular dementia, Lewy body disease, and multiple sclerosis; psychiatric disorders such as major depressive disorder, bipolar affective disorder, and attention deficit and cerebrovascular disorders (Fig. 1 and Table 1).
Table 1
Evolution over time of the classic HPN features in the INPH group
Variable
n
%
M
SD
Range
Age at onset of symptoms (years)
61
 
73.2
6.7
 
Time between start and STT (months)
61
 
27.7
26.2
 
Age at STT
61
 
75.5
6.5
58–86
Educational level—years
61
 
11.5
4.9
2–23
Male
31
 
50.8
  
 < One year with symptoms
15
24.6
   
 > One year with symptoms
46
75.4
   
 < Two years with symptoms
35
57.4
   
 > Two years with symptoms
26
42.6
   
Gait
61
100
   
Cognition
57
90
   
Sphincter
45
69.8
   
One symptom (gait)
4
6.7
   
Two symptoms (*)
21
35
   
Complete triad
35
58.3
   
Values described in mean (M), standard deviation (SD) and percentages (%). STT Serial Tap Test, n number of individuals; ≤ : values equal to or less than the cut-off point; > : values higher than the cut-off point; * gait apraxia associated with cognitive or sphincter symptoms
Individuals with a history of depression under control for more than three years or a history of mild TBI without permanent sequelae (cognitive or motor) were recruited (Table 2).
Table 2
Associated clinical conditions of the group with INPH
Variable
Sample (N)
n
%
Neurological eventsa
 
16
26.22
TBI
 
8
 
Stroke
 
6
 
SAH
 
1
 
TIA
 
1
 
Hypertension
57
36
63.2
History of Depression
60
15
25
Diabetes
61
15
24.6
Dyslipidemia
61
11
18
Hypothyroidism
60
7
11.7
Smoking
60
3
5
Data described in original sample number (N), number (n) and percentages (%); TBI traumatic brain injury, SAH subarachnoid hemorrhage, TIA transient ischemic attack. All individuals in the group with stroke [n = 6] presented a time interval between the stroke and STT greater than three years. Three subjects presented lower-impact ischemic stroke without sequelae. One individual had three episodes: in the first he became unable to wear his slippers, in the second there was loss of peripheral vision, and finally right hemiparesis in the third

Control population

A control group was recruited from the community (patient’s and or hospital staff’s families and from an aged Baptist Church group). They were submitted to an interview about medical, scholarship and functional history, besides current health. Individuals suspected of neurological or psychiatric diseases were not included. An initial group of 57 individuals was selected. Of these, three subjects were excluded because of depression and brain trauma in the preceding 3 years.

Study final population

The remaining 66 INPH individuals and 54 control subjects underwent propensity score matching and both groups were homogenized according to age, gender, and educational level. This led to the exclusion of five individuals from the INPH group and 12 individuals from the control group. Data of 61 individuals from the INPH group and 42 from the control group were then compared (Fig. 2).
Procedures

Tap Test

Patients were admitted to the hospital for two to three days. Two lumbar punctures were performed at intervals of at least 24 h apart (minimum 24 h and maximum 28 h, to guarantee total LCR turnover) and 30 mL of CSF (M: 26 ± 1.2), was withdrawn in each, as described by Adams [1]. CSF opening pressure was measured and a CSF sample was sent for laboratory analysis. The first LP was preceded by cognitive and gait examinations performed by a team of three neuropsychologists and four neurologists, respectively. After being examined for excluding post-puncture pain or headache [9] and between two and six hours after each LP, INPH subjects were tested again for both gait and cognitive examinations. Gait and cognition were never assessed within an interval shorter than one hour after the LP (M = 4,4 ± 2,5). Neither persistent headache nor persistent pain after lumbar puncture were reported by individuals in this series, and further information regarding this issue can be found in a recent paper of our group [15].
To minimize interindividual bias, the neuropsychology team was constantly trained on study procedures. Furthermore, study data were discussed weekly and investigators remained blind to investigation subjects' diagnosis during study procedures. Gait protocol has been described elsewhere [16] and will not be discussed in this paper.

Neuropsychological examination

The neuropsychological assessment was performed during hospitalization. The average duration of the cognitive protocol was one hour and thirty minutes. Some data was not collected from patients with visual or motor impairment. The cognitive tests used were: Orientation (self-awareness, orientation in place, time and current context). Mental Control (usual and unusual activities: to speak the days of the week, the months of the year, to count (e.g.: 1–20) and after this, all the same activities are repeated in backward), FAR-COWA (The examinee must produce orally as many words as possible beginning with a specified letter during one minute (F-A-R) and thereafter he is asked to produce as many animal names as possible within a one-minute interval); Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT): This is an episodic memory test: a 15-word list is read aloud (A list) for five consecutive trials (A1–A5). After trial A5, a distractor 15-word list is presented once and the examinee needs to learn it (B1); immediately after this, the examinee is asked to recall the A list (A6—immediate recall) and twenty minutes later again (A7—delayed recall); Rey Complex Figure Test (CFT): A visuospatial memory test: the examinee needs to learn a new complex figure by copying it. After a three-minute interval, he is asked to reproduce the figure as similar as possible without looking at it and thirty minutes later again); Digit Span Test (WMS-R): A two-step working memory test: 1—the examinee is asked to repeat series of numbers in the same order that they are presented by the examiner; the quantity of digits increases progressively (digit span forward); 2—the examinee is asked to repeat similar series but in backward (digit span backward). In our protocol both parts are scored separately; Stroop test (University of Victoria Version): This is an attention test: the examinee is presented to three cards. In the first card, there are 24 colored dots (blue, red, yellow, green). The subject is required to name the colors in which the stimuli are printed as fast as possible. In the second part, neutral words (when, where, what) are printed in the same colors of the dots and the subject needs to name the colors while ignoring the words. In the third part, the words blue, red, yellow and green are printed in conflicting colors (e.g.: the word “blue” printed in red color). Individuals are asked to name the colors printed again while avoid reading the words (name of colors). Scores are calculated based on the speed to perform the tasks and on the number of errors only in the third part; Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE): This is a 30-point scoring test composed of “Introduction” section—items of orientation, memory and attention (21 points); “Materials” section—items of naming, follow verbal and written commands, writing a sentence and drawing a figure with intersections (9 points); Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB): This is a brief battery consisting of six subtests: similarities, lexical fluency, motor series, conflicting instructions, Go-No-Go test and prehension behavior). Each subtest can be scored from 0 to 3 points. The score is based on the sum of the six test scores.
To avoid a learning effect, some tests were applied in up to three different versions. The tests used in this study are among the most cited in INPH literature [17, 18].

Matching procedure

Patients with IHPH and control subjects were matched using propensity scores (PS). Three variables were selected to estimate PS: age, sex and formal education. The propensity score was calculated using logistic regression and patients with IHPH were matched with controls using the nearest neighbor technique with a predefined caliper of 0.2 and considering the ratio 1:1. This matching procedure was performed using the MatchIt package for R.16.

Statistics

A propensity score model was used to homogenize the INPH and the control group considering a logistic regression model and conditioning the variables of age, sex and formal education.
To analyze the effect of STT on cognitive test results of the INPH group in comparison with the control group, Z scores were calculated for each INPH patient regarding mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) of the control group. The Z scores of variables whose improvements were equivalent to smaller values, such as run time number of errors, variables dots, words and colors, and errors of the Stroop test, were multiplied by − 1 (inverted). Thus, in all variables, the highest values of the Z score corresponded to the participants’ best results.
Results of quantitative variables were described by means and standard deviations. Categorical variables were presented in frequencies and percentages. Comparison between the INPH and control groups, concerning categorical variables, was conducted using Fisher's Exact Test. Comparison of either two disease-defined groups or clinical factors, relative to quantitative variables, was conducted using either Student's t-test for independent samples or the non-parametric Mann–Whitney test. More than two groups were compared considering the nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test. Normality condition was evaluated through the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Values of p < 0.05 indicate statistical significance. Data were analyzed using the IBM Corp. Released 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
After propensity score matching, from the 66 subjects with INPH and the 54 control subjects initially recruited, 61 INPH subjects and 42 control subjects remained for the statistical analysis. These groups were balanced regarding age (p = 0.056), sex (p = 0.231) and educational level (p = 0.549).
Statistical analysis was supervised by a professional statistician.

Results

Regarding cognitive performance before the first LP, the INPH group scored lower than controls in every test except for the forward Digit Span Test. Values before and after the LP were also compared with control group scores (Table 3). The INPH group presented a similar score to the control group in the forward Digit Span Test in all steps of the serial drainage test. However, other measures of verbal memory, such as RAVLT—A1 and B1, improved after LP.
Table 3
INPH Pre-LP x post-LP1 and pre x post-LP2 cognitive scores, and control group cognitive scores
Variable
n
BLP
  
ALP1
  
ALP2
 
CG
 
M (SD)
p*
n
M (SD)
p**
n
M (SD)
p***
n
M (SD)
OR
61
9.6 (2.7)
 < 0.001
59
9.9 (2.8)
 < 0.001
55
9.9 (2.9)
0.001
42
11.7 (0.8)
MC
60
6.1 (2.3)
0.002
57
6.2 (2.2)
0.002
55
6.2 (2.2)
0.002
42
7.5 (1.1)
DS-For
59
5.4 (2.1)
0.707
55
5.7 (2.2)
0.885
54
5.7 (2.2)
0.606
42
5.6 (1.7)
DS-Back
59
3.3 (1.8)
0.003
55
3.4 (1.9)
0.026
54
3.4 (1.9)
0.015
39
4.2 (1.3)
FAR
58
20.7 (12.1)
 < 0.001
54
22.4 (13.0)
0.008
52
23.9 (13.1)
0.051
41
29.6 (10.6)
Animals
58
9.3 (4.9)
 < 0.001
53
9.2 (4.7)
 < 0.001
53
9.6 (5.3)
 < 0.001
41
13.9 (4.1)
A1
58
3.1 (1.4)
0.014
54
3.4 (1.7)
0.093
52
3.4 (1.6)
0.146
42
4.0 (1.8)
A5
57
6.0 (3.1)
 < 0.001
54
6.6 (2.9)
 < 0.001
52
6.0 (2.8)
 < 0.001
42
9.6 (2.5)
Total
57
24.5 (10.4)
 < 0.001
54
26.6 (10.5)
 < 0.001
52
25.3 (10.6)
 < 0.001
42
37.0 (10.3)
B1
56
2.7 (1.7)
0.010
53
2.7 (1.5)
0.007
51
3.3 (2.0)
0.293
42
3.7 (1.7)
A6
59
2.7 (1.7)
0.010
55
2.7 (1.5)
0.007
55
3.3 (2.0)
0.293
42
3.7 (1.7)
A7
58
3.2 (2.7)
 < 0.001
56
3.0 (2.8)
 < 0.001
55
2.5 (2.5)
 < 0.001
42
6.7 (2.9)
S-Dotsa
53
31.0 (24.8)
 < 0.001
53
25.0 (19.3)
0.038
49
23.1 (16.8)
0.014
41
17.4 (5.3)
S-Wordb
52
42.4 (31.6)
 < 0.001
52
34.9 (25.8)
0.023
47
30.0 (19.0)
0.080
41
25.1 (9.1)
S-Colorsc
52
58.6 (32.4)
0.003
49
52.8 (42.7)
0.255
47
46.1 (26.4)
0.479
39
42.8 (18.9)
S-Errord
46
8.1 (8.8)
 < 0.001
46
6.2 (7.1)
0.002
39
4.7 (6.0)
0.092
41
2.3 (2.9)
CFT-cop
34
24.0 (9.4)
0.016
33
22.5 (9.5)
0.002
29
24.2 (9.4)
0.047
42
28.7 (7.1)
CFT- IR
34
7.5 (6.0)
0.003
31
10.0 (8.1)
0.159
27
11.5 (8.8)
0.389
42
12.2 (6.8)
CFT- DR
27
5.4 (4.5)
 < 0.001
26
10.9 (11.8)
0.098
20
9.8 (9.0)
0.108
39
11.8 (6.0)
All values described in mean (M) and standard deviation (SD); n: number of individuals; BLP: step before lumbar punctures; ALP1: step after the first lumbar puncture; ALP2: step after the second lumbar puncture. Source tests: OR: orientation; MC: mental control; DS-FOR: forward digit span; DS-BACK: backwards digit span; FAR: verbal fluency test; RAVLT: Rey auditory verbal learning test; S-Dotsa: Stroop Test—dots step; S-Wordb: Stroop test – words step; S-Colorsc: Stroop test- colors step; S-Errord: Stroop test – number of errors in the colors step; CFT-cop: Copy of Rey complex figure test; CFT-IR: Immediate reproduction of Rey complex figure test; CFT-DR: Delayed Reproduction of Rey complex figure test; MMSE Mini mental state examination, FAB frontal evaluation battery. P values: * non-parametric Mann–Whitney test, p < 0.05
The INPH group scored lower than controls in FAB and MMSE scales before LP and after the two LP remained lower than those of the control group (Mann–Whitney, p = 0.017 and p = 0.034, respectively).
Table 4 shows the comparisons among the three steps of the cognitive test for all tests applied during STT.
Table 4
Comparison of the three assessment moments of the INPH group
Original test
Variable
n
BLP
ALP1
ALP2
p*
Mean ± SD
(Z Score)
OR
Orientation
54
− 2.7 ± 3.5
− 2.4 ± 3.7
− 2.2 ± 3.8
0.018
MC
Mental control
53
− 1.3 ± 2.2
− 1.1 ± 2.1
− 1.2 ± 2.1
0.156
DS
Forward
51
− 0.1 ± 1.3
0.1 ± 1.3
0.1 ± 1.3
0.327
Backward
51
− 0.7 ± 1.4
− 0.6 ± 1.4
− 0.6 ± 1.4
0.600
VF
FAR
28
− 1.2 ± 1.6
− 1 ± 1.8
− 0.9 ± 1.5
0.053
Animals
49
− 1.1 ± 1.2
− 1.2 ± 1.1
− 1 ± 1.3
0.753
RAVLT
A1
50
− 0.5 ± 0.8
− 0.5 ± 0.8
− 0.4 ± 0.9
0.490
A5
50
− 1.5 ± 1.2
− 1.2 ± 1.1
− 1.5 ± 1.1
0.306
Total A1−A5
50
− 1.2 ± 1
− 1.1 ± 0.9
− 1.2 ± 1
0.113
B1
49
− 0.6 ± 1
− 0.6 ± 0.8
− 0.3 ± 1.2
0.024
A6
51
− 1.2 ± 0.9
− 1.3 ± 0.9
− 1.5 ± 0.8
0.081
A7
51
− 1.3 ± 0.9
− 1.4 ± 0.9
− 1.5 ± 0.8
0.048
STROOP
S-Dots
45
− 2.2 ± 4.5
− 1.4 ± 3.3
− 1.2 ± 3.1
 < 0.001
S-Words
43
1.5 ± 3.3
1 ± 2.5
0.7 ± 2.1
0.031
S-Colors
42
− 0.6 ± 1.6
− 0.5 ± 2
− 0.3 ± 1.4
0.541
S-Errors
36
− 1.9 ± 3.1
− 1.4 ± 2.4
− 0.9 ± 2.2
0.010
CFT
CFT-Cop
26
− 0.6 ± 1.3
− 0.8 ± 1.4
− 0.8 ± 1.3
0.254
CFT-IR
24
− 0.6 ± 0.9
− 0.4 ± 1.1
0 ± 1.3
0.006
CFT-DR
15
− 1 ± 0.8
0 ± 2.3
− 0.4 ± 1.4
0.167
MMSE
MMSE
48
− 1.9 ± 2.6
− 1.6 ± 2.8
− 1.4 ± 3
0.021
FAB
FAB
28
− 1.2 ± 1.6
− 1 ± 1.8
− 0.9 ± 1.5
0.188
All values converted and described in Z scores. SD standard deviation, n number of individuals, BLP step before lumbar punctures, ALP1 step after the first lumbar puncture, post ALP2 step after the second lumbar puncture. Source tests: OR orientation, MC mental control, DS digit span, VF verbal fluency test, FAR lexical fluency; Animals: semantic fluency; RAVLT: Rey auditory verbal learning test; S-Dots: Stroop Test—dots step; S-Word: Stroop test – words step; S-Colors: Stroop test- colors step; S-Error: Stroop test – number of errors in the colors step; CFT-cop: Copy of Rey complex figure test; CFT-IR: Immediate reproduction of Rey complex figure test; CFT-DR: Delayed Reproduction of Rey complex figure test; MMSE mini mental state examination, FAB frontal evaluation battery. P values: * Non-parametric Friedman test, p < 0.05
Table 5 summarizes all variables with any significant improvement among the three testing steps, except for RAVLT-A7 (comparison between the moment before lumbar puncture and after the second lumbar puncture), which presented worsening.
Table 5
Analysis of the differences of three evaluation moments of the INPH group
Cognitive variable
Difference between steps
p
A* scores
B* scores
C* scores
pre-LP x post-LP1
pre-LP x post-LP2
post-LP1 x post-LP2
OR—orientation
0.121
0.004
0.177
0.018
MMSE
0.031
0.008
0.614
0.021
RAVLT—B1
0.711
0.028
0.011
0.024
RAVLT—A7*
0.153
0.014
0.290
0.048
ST—dots
p < 0.001
p < 0.001
0.855
p < 0.001
ST—words
0.069
0.010
0.422
0.031
ST—errors
0.241
0.002
0.053
0.010
CFT- IR
0.111
0.001
0.066
0.006
P values: * Non-parametric Friedman test, p < 0.05; Post-hoc analysis with comparisons of groups. LP: lumbar puncture. Variable with its respective test of origin: OR: orientation; MMSE: Mini Mental State Examination; RAVLT-B1: B list of the Rey verbal learning test; RAVLT-A7: late recall [20 min after the list A6] of the Rey verbal learning test; ST-dots: first step [dots] of the Stroop test in the University of Victoria version; ST-words: second step [words] of the Stroop test in the University of Victoria version; ST-errors: numbers of errors in the third step [colors] of the Stroop test in the University of Vitoria version; CFT-IR: Immediate reproduction [after 3 min] of the Rey complex figure test
An additional analysis was performed considering the previous history of associated clinical conditions of the INPH group, even if these conditions were not present for more than three years before the research period (Table 2). No differences were observed between subgroups with a history of previous depression, traumatic brain injury, or arterial hypertension. The stroke group presented better performance than the group without stroke in orientation (comparison between the moment before lumbar puncture and after first lumbar puncture) and the B1 memory RAVLT item (comparison between moment after first lumbar puncture and after second lumbar puncture; p = 0.034 and p = 0.047, respectively). All stroke individuals presented INPH symptoms for less than a year and did not report any cognitive permanent sequelae after the stroke event. Participants with diabetes presented greater mental slowness when compared with nondiabetic individuals (p = 0.029), even after the second LP.

Discussion

INPH is one of the many diseases that can affect both motor and non-motor circuitry of the basal ganglia, and cause motor, autonomic, cognitive and behavioral symptoms [19]. Our study aimed to focus only on cognitive-behavioral manifestations of INPH. The STT protocol was carried out at a hospital ward, avoiding the need for commuting, an aspect that contributed to its acceptance by patients and family. Hospitalization allowed all stages of the procedure to be controlled regarding external interferences on test results.
Serial drainage testing resulted in lower morbidity than continuous lumbar drainage [8, 20]. Moreover, the second LP may increase the sensitivity of STT once detects more changes in cognitive variables, thus improving the method.
INPH subjects had lower performance than controls before LP in all tests regarding selective attention measures (words and colors in the Stroop test), distraction resistance (errors in the Stroop test), immediate and late visual memory (CFT). Similar results were described by Katzen et al. [21]. The exceptions to this finding in our study were the Digit Span Test. The fact that the Digit Span Scores were calculated forward and backward separately, probably triggered the differences regarding other studies [19, 22, 23], perhaps due to a ceiling effect in control individuals. However, other measures of supraspan, such as A1 and B1 of RAVLT, showed improvement.
The scores of the INPH group before LP were lower than the control group scores in both screening scales (MMSE and FAB) as previously observed by Katzen et al. [21] and Saito et al. [23]. Both scores remained lower after LPs, which reinforces the finding of severely compromised cognitive function in this population.
Eight cognitive items improved along with the STT, but the most paradoxical result was the decline of the RAVLT-A7 item, contrasting with the improvement seen in other tests. Serial Tap Test is an extensive protocol composed of several tasks applied to an elderly population. RAVLT-A7 item is the last phase carried out on the second day of examination and, for this reason, probably fatigue may have contributed to this result.
Significant improvement in A* scores (Table 5) probably reflect an enhancement in alertness [24]. The complex relationships between alertness and attention have been adequately discussed in the literature [25].
It seems likely that both the enhancement in alertness and the decompression of frontostriatal circuits promote a gradual improvement in many cognitive aspects, especially in the dots and words variables of the Stroop test, in which subjects reacted faster after LP. Participants also showed improvement in their ability to inhibit impulsive responses and to resist distractions (errors in the Stroop Test) [26]. Perhaps enhanced spatiotemporal perception revealed by the orientation test is also secondary to a better state of alertness. Isik et al. [27] performed serial punctures in the INPH patients group (mean duration interval of 7.4 ± 5.7 months between the first and second LP and a mean duration of 8.5 ± 3.8 months between second and third punctures). Each time, they tested these patients before LP and 24 h after. They found not a significant difference in the Stroop Test comparisons, at the first and second puncture moments. This result contrasts with what we found out. The first explanation is that our time interval between LP and the neuropsychological assessment was shorter (mean 4 h). Although INPH physiopathology is not so far well explained, immediate and delayed mechanisms may be involved. A decompression effect may be immediately releasing attentional circuits, promoting a cognitive enhancement in the alertness status. This aspect, however, does not appear to be the only mechanism involved in INPH cognitive dysfunction.
Several other hypotheses were formulated to explain the complex pathophysiology of this disease, some of which involve cerebral parenchyma and CNS blood vessels, accumulation of toxic metabolites in CSF, and transependymal CSF permeation with cell and axonal damage [15, 19, 2832].
A similar effect on step A1 and B1 of RAVLT could be expected due to this gain in alertness since the structure of the two lists of words is similar. Despite the improvement seen in B1 (p = 0.024), the same was not found in A1 (p = 0.490). The structure of RAVLT may explain this difference since B1 is presented as a distraction element between the learning curve (A1-A5) and the immediate recall (A6) [33], whereas A1 is the first list presented in the test.
According to the theory of interference [1214], forgetfulness can be understood as the interference of one memory over another. In RAVLT, a proactive interference (PI) is when prior learning affects later learning, but also a retroactive interference (RI) can occur, in which further learning affects the recovery of previously learned information. Thus, the improvement seen in B1 reflects a decrease in PI, but, in contrast, there was no change in RI (A6 – p = 0.081).
Time spent on the STT and the differences in the A*, B*, and C* scores indicated cognitive evolution over the two days of examination. Time reported being required for cognitive improvement after LP varies among authors from 30–60 min to a week [8, 3436]. However, all cognitive studies regarding TT screened for changes after a single puncture [4, 6, 8, 9, 3639]. The present study is so far the first to systematically use serial lumbar punctures and systematically retest cognition over steps and time. Significant improvement in several cognitive domains in such a short time interval, compared with those reported in previous literature, suggests that the changes detected were a result of repeated punctures rather than merely the passage of time. Therefore, according to our results, mental speed, the earliest improved function after LP, continues improving after the second LP. It is likely that other skills may improve because of mental speed increasing, e.g., phonetic, and lexical verbal fluency tasks. These kinds of tests depend on language and executive functions. NPHI is not presumed to affect directly cortical functions as language fluency, but to mechanically compress the periventricular frontostriatal circuits and to cause transependymal CSF leakage and parenchymal edema, affecting language pathways speed. Thereafter, the progressive release of this circuitry may ameliorate its functioning.
Some limitations of this study should be considered. No retest in the control group was performed. Comparisons between the INPH group after LP and the control group after mental function re-testing would clearly define whether the changes observed in INPH subjects were not a learning effect (LE). LE may depend on individuals’ characteristics as age and performance baseline [40]. A recent review [41] identified a lower LE in individuals with dementia as compared to normal aging individuals [4245]. Our series of INPH individuals scored significantly below the normal aging group in all tests (except for the “digit span backward” subtest), a finding congruent with a cognitive decline. A single study about LE in INPH was published by Solana et al. [40] reported no learning effect in the INPH population when reapplying the same cognitive tests over four consecutive days. Our testing protocol started before Solana et al. [40] publication and has slight differences from their protocol, but the tests we have used are among the most used in INPH literature and there is a vast literature about patients' performance on them [17, 19, 21, 22, 35].
LE may depend on test characteristics as well, for example, a short-time-interval can result in a strong LE [40], and we sought for alternative testing to reduce the influence of this variable.
Lack of data on depression was also a gap in the study. However, self-evaluative scales have a limited effect on this population due to difficulties in differentiating depressive symptoms from frontal dysfunction. Kito et al. [46] observed that apathy is the most common neuropsychiatric disorder in this population and that this symptom has a high correlation with cognitive symptoms of the triad. Apathy is considered a symptom of somnolence-sopor-coma disorder (SSCD) [24, 47], as well as the emotional asthenic syndrome or apathetic-abulic syndrome [41]. The correlation between apathy and executive dysfunction has been attributed to the association of this symptom with INPH dysfunctional brain areas, such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and thalamus [48].
Another drawback is the lack of follow-up data after CSF shunting, and there is no doubt that this information is desired. To fill this gap, longitudinal information is being gathered to confirm the long-term improvement of the aforementioned cognitive aspects and their consistency over time.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Declarations

The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Neurological Institute of Curitiba in Humans Research for having followed the principles of the Helsinki Convention and its later amendments, as well as Brazilian guidelines disposed by resolution No. 466/2012, all individuals were adequately consented. The committee’s reference number is CAAE: 26239614.0.0000.5227.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Adams RD, Fisher CM, Hakim S, Ojemann RG, Sweet WH. Symptomatic occult hydrocephalus with normal cerebrospinal-fluid pressure. N Engl J Med. 1965;273:117–26.PubMedCrossRef Adams RD, Fisher CM, Hakim S, Ojemann RG, Sweet WH. Symptomatic occult hydrocephalus with normal cerebrospinal-fluid pressure. N Engl J Med. 1965;273:117–26.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson M. Normal pressure hydrocephalus. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1986;293(6551):837–8.CrossRef Anderson M. Normal pressure hydrocephalus. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1986;293(6551):837–8.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Relkin N, Klinge P, Bergsneider M, Black PM. Diagnosing idiopathic normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 2005;57(Suppl 3):4–16. Relkin N, Klinge P, Bergsneider M, Black PM. Diagnosing idiopathic normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 2005;57(Suppl 3):4–16.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Wikkelsö C, Andersson H, Blomstrand C, Lindqvist G, Svendsen P. Predictive value of the cerebrospinal fluid tap-test. Acta Neurol Scand. 1986;73:566–73.PubMedCrossRef Wikkelsö C, Andersson H, Blomstrand C, Lindqvist G, Svendsen P. Predictive value of the cerebrospinal fluid tap-test. Acta Neurol Scand. 1986;73:566–73.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Marmarou A, Bergsneider M, Klinge P, Relkin N, Black PM. The value of supplemental prognostic tests for the preoperative assessment of idiopathic normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 2005;57(Suppl 3):S17-28.PubMed Marmarou A, Bergsneider M, Klinge P, Relkin N, Black PM. The value of supplemental prognostic tests for the preoperative assessment of idiopathic normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 2005;57(Suppl 3):S17-28.PubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Ishikawa M, Hashimoto M, Mori E, Kuwana N, Kazui H. The value of the cerebrospinal fluid tap test for predicting shunt effectiveness in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Fluids Barriers CNS. 2012;9(1):1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ishikawa M, Hashimoto M, Mori E, Kuwana N, Kazui H. The value of the cerebrospinal fluid tap test for predicting shunt effectiveness in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Fluids Barriers CNS. 2012;9(1):1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Haan J, Thomeer RT. Predictive value of temporary external lumbar drainage in normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 1988;22(2):388–91.PubMedCrossRef Haan J, Thomeer RT. Predictive value of temporary external lumbar drainage in normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 1988;22(2):388–91.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Walchenbach R, Geiger E, Thommer R, Vanneste J. The value of temporary external lumbar CSF drainage in predicting the outcome of shunting on normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;72(4):503–6.PubMedPubMedCentral Walchenbach R, Geiger E, Thommer R, Vanneste J. The value of temporary external lumbar CSF drainage in predicting the outcome of shunting on normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;72(4):503–6.PubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Virhammar J, Cesarini KG, Laurell K. The CSF tap test in normal pressure hydrocephalus: evaluation time, reliability and the influence of pain. Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(2):271–6.PubMedCrossRef Virhammar J, Cesarini KG, Laurell K. The CSF tap test in normal pressure hydrocephalus: evaluation time, reliability and the influence of pain. Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(2):271–6.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Queenan BN, Ryan TJ, Gazzaniga MS, Gallistel CR. On the research of time past: the hunt for the substrate of memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2017;1396(1):108–25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Queenan BN, Ryan TJ, Gazzaniga MS, Gallistel CR. On the research of time past: the hunt for the substrate of memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2017;1396(1):108–25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Muller GE, Schuman NF. Experimentelle beiträge zur untersuchung des gedächtmisses. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie Am J Psychol. 1984;6(2):299–301. Muller GE, Schuman NF. Experimentelle beiträge zur untersuchung des gedächtmisses. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie Am J Psychol. 1984;6(2):299–301.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Pergher GK, Stein LM. Compreendendo o esquecimento: teorias clássicas e seus fundamentos experimentais. Psicol USP. 2003;14(1):129–55.CrossRef Pergher GK, Stein LM. Compreendendo o esquecimento: teorias clássicas e seus fundamentos experimentais. Psicol USP. 2003;14(1):129–55.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Wixted JT. The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:235–69.PubMedCrossRef Wixted JT. The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:235–69.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Martins RT, Toson B, Souza RKM, Kowacs PA. Post-dural puncture headache incidence after cerebrospinal fluid aspiration. A prospective observational study. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2020;78(4):187–92.PubMedCrossRef Martins RT, Toson B, Souza RKM, Kowacs PA. Post-dural puncture headache incidence after cerebrospinal fluid aspiration. A prospective observational study. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2020;78(4):187–92.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Souza RKM, Rocha SFB, Martins RT, Kowcas PA, Ramina R. Gait in normal pressure hydrocephalus: characteristics and effects of the CSF tap test. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2018;76(5):324–31.PubMedCrossRef Souza RKM, Rocha SFB, Martins RT, Kowcas PA, Ramina R. Gait in normal pressure hydrocephalus: characteristics and effects of the CSF tap test. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2018;76(5):324–31.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Benejam, B. Poca MA, Junqué C, Solana E, Sahuquillo J. Cognitive impairment in normal pressure hydrocephalus (NPH): a proposal for clinical evaluation protocol. Neurocirugia (Astur). 2008;19(4):309–21. Benejam, B. Poca MA, Junqué C, Solana E, Sahuquillo J. Cognitive impairment in normal pressure hydrocephalus (NPH): a proposal for clinical evaluation protocol. Neurocirugia (Astur). 2008;19(4):309–21.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiefer M, Unterberg A. The differential diagnosis and treatment of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2012;109(1–2):15–25.PubMedPubMedCentral Kiefer M, Unterberg A. The differential diagnosis and treatment of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2012;109(1–2):15–25.PubMedPubMedCentral
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Bugalho P, Alves L, Miguel R, Ribeiro O. Profile of cognitive dysfunction and relation with gait disturbance in Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2014;118:83–8.PubMedCrossRef Bugalho P, Alves L, Miguel R, Ribeiro O. Profile of cognitive dysfunction and relation with gait disturbance in Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2014;118:83–8.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Kazui H, Mori E, Ohkawa S, et al. Predictors of the disappearance of triad symptoms in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus after shunt surgery. J Neurol Sci. 2013;328(1–2):64–9.PubMedCrossRef Kazui H, Mori E, Ohkawa S, et al. Predictors of the disappearance of triad symptoms in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus after shunt surgery. J Neurol Sci. 2013;328(1–2):64–9.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Katzen H, Ravdin LD, Assuras S, et al. Postshunt cognitive and functional improvement in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 2011;68(2):416–9.PubMedCrossRef Katzen H, Ravdin LD, Assuras S, et al. Postshunt cognitive and functional improvement in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery. 2011;68(2):416–9.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Gleichgerrcht E, Cervio A, Salvat J, Loffredo AR, Vita L, Roca M, Torralva T, Manes F. Executive function improvement in normal pressure hydrocephalus following shunt surgery. Behav Neurol. 2009;21(3):181–5.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gleichgerrcht E, Cervio A, Salvat J, Loffredo AR, Vita L, Roca M, Torralva T, Manes F. Executive function improvement in normal pressure hydrocephalus following shunt surgery. Behav Neurol. 2009;21(3):181–5.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Saito M, Nishio Y, Kanno S, Uchiyama M, et al. Cognitive profile of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2011;1(1):202–11.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Saito M, Nishio Y, Kanno S, Uchiyama M, et al. Cognitive profile of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2011;1(1):202–11.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Tullberg M, Hellström P, Piechnik SK, Starmark JE, Wikkelsö C. Impaired wakefulness is associated with reduced anterior cingulate CBF in patients with normal pressure hydrocephalus. Acta Neurol Scand. 2004;110(5):322–30.PubMedCrossRef Tullberg M, Hellström P, Piechnik SK, Starmark JE, Wikkelsö C. Impaired wakefulness is associated with reduced anterior cingulate CBF in patients with normal pressure hydrocephalus. Acta Neurol Scand. 2004;110(5):322–30.PubMedCrossRef
25.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Brandelero V, Toni PM. Study of validity of Stroop color and word test for inhibitory control. Psicol Argum. 2015;33(80):282–97. Brandelero V, Toni PM. Study of validity of Stroop color and word test for inhibitory control. Psicol Argum. 2015;33(80):282–97.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Isik AT, Kaya D, Ates Bulut E, Dokuzlar O, Soysal P. The outcomes of serial cerebrospinal fluid removal in elderly patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Clin Interv Aging. 2019;14:2063–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Isik AT, Kaya D, Ates Bulut E, Dokuzlar O, Soysal P. The outcomes of serial cerebrospinal fluid removal in elderly patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Clin Interv Aging. 2019;14:2063–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley WG, Whittemore AR, Watanabe AS, Davis SJ, Teresi LM, Homyak M. Association of deep white matter infarction with chronic communicating hydrocephalus: implications regarding the possible origin of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1991;12(1):31–9.PubMedPubMedCentral Bradley WG, Whittemore AR, Watanabe AS, Davis SJ, Teresi LM, Homyak M. Association of deep white matter infarction with chronic communicating hydrocephalus: implications regarding the possible origin of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1991;12(1):31–9.PubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Tullberg M, Hultin L, Ekholm S, Månsson JE, Fredman P, Wikkelsø C. White matter changes in normal pressure hydrocephalus and Binswanger disease: specificity, predictive value and correlations to axonal degeneration and demyelination. Acta Neurol Scand. 2002;105(6):417–26.PubMedCrossRef Tullberg M, Hultin L, Ekholm S, Månsson JE, Fredman P, Wikkelsø C. White matter changes in normal pressure hydrocephalus and Binswanger disease: specificity, predictive value and correlations to axonal degeneration and demyelination. Acta Neurol Scand. 2002;105(6):417–26.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Takeuchi T, Goto H, Izaki K, Tamura S, et al. Pathophysiology of cerebral circulatory disorders in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurol Med Chir (Tokyo). 2007;47(7):299–306.CrossRef Takeuchi T, Goto H, Izaki K, Tamura S, et al. Pathophysiology of cerebral circulatory disorders in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurol Med Chir (Tokyo). 2007;47(7):299–306.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Bateman GA. The pathophysiology of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus: cerebral ischemia or altered venous hemodynamics? AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2008;29(1):198–203.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Bateman GA. The pathophysiology of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus: cerebral ischemia or altered venous hemodynamics? AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2008;29(1):198–203.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen YF, Wang YH, Hsiao JK, et al. Normal pressure hydrocephalus: cerebral hemodynamic, metabolism measurement, discharge score, and long-term outcome. Surg Neurol. 2008;70(Suppl 1):69–77.CrossRef Chen YF, Wang YH, Hsiao JK, et al. Normal pressure hydrocephalus: cerebral hemodynamic, metabolism measurement, discharge score, and long-term outcome. Surg Neurol. 2008;70(Suppl 1):69–77.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary. New York: Oxford University. 1998;2:1240. Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary. New York: Oxford University. 1998;2:1240.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahlon B, Sundbärg G, Rehncrona S. Comparison between the lumbar infusion and CSF tap tests to predict outcome after shunt surgery in suspected normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73(6):721–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kahlon B, Sundbärg G, Rehncrona S. Comparison between the lumbar infusion and CSF tap tests to predict outcome after shunt surgery in suspected normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73(6):721–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmidt H, Elster J, Eckert I, Wiefek J, et al. Cognitive functions after spinal tap in patients with normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol. 2014;261(12):2344–50.PubMedCrossRef Schmidt H, Elster J, Eckert I, Wiefek J, et al. Cognitive functions after spinal tap in patients with normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol. 2014;261(12):2344–50.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Yamada S, Ishikawa M, Miyajima M, et al. Disease duration: the key to accurate CSF tap test in iNPH. Acta Neurol Scand. 2017;135(2):189–96.PubMedCrossRef Yamada S, Ishikawa M, Miyajima M, et al. Disease duration: the key to accurate CSF tap test in iNPH. Acta Neurol Scand. 2017;135(2):189–96.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasceno BP, Carelli EF, Honorato DC, Facure JJ. The predictive value of cerebrospinal fluid tap-test in normal pressure hydrocephalus. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 1997;55(2):179–85.PubMedCrossRef Damasceno BP, Carelli EF, Honorato DC, Facure JJ. The predictive value of cerebrospinal fluid tap-test in normal pressure hydrocephalus. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 1997;55(2):179–85.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Wikkelsø C, Hellström P, Klinge PM, Tans JT, European iNPH Multicentre Study Group. The European iNPH Multicentre Study on the predictive values of resistance to CSF outflow and the CSF Tap Test in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84(5):562–8. Wikkelsø C, Hellström P, Klinge PM, Tans JT, European iNPH Multicentre Study Group. The European iNPH Multicentre Study on the predictive values of resistance to CSF outflow and the CSF Tap Test in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84(5):562–8.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams MA, Malm J. Diagnosis and treatment of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Continuum (Minneap Minn). 2016;22(2):579–99.PubMedPubMedCentral Williams MA, Malm J. Diagnosis and treatment of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Continuum (Minneap Minn). 2016;22(2):579–99.PubMedPubMedCentral
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Solana E, Poca MA, Sahuquillo J, Benejam B, Junqué C, Dronavalli M. Cognitive and motor improvement after retesting in normal-pressure hydrocephalus: a real change or merely a learning effect? J Neurosurg. 2010;112(2):399–409.PubMedCrossRef Solana E, Poca MA, Sahuquillo J, Benejam B, Junqué C, Dronavalli M. Cognitive and motor improvement after retesting in normal-pressure hydrocephalus: a real change or merely a learning effect? J Neurosurg. 2010;112(2):399–409.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Jutten RJ, Grandoit E, Foldi NS, Sikkes SAM, Jones RN, Choi SE, Lamar ML, Louden DKN, Rich J, Tommet D, Crane PK, Rabin LA. Lower practice effects as a marker of cognitive performance and dementia risk: A literature review. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2020;12(1):2055. Jutten RJ, Grandoit E, Foldi NS, Sikkes SAM, Jones RN, Choi SE, Lamar ML, Louden DKN, Rich J, Tommet D, Crane PK, Rabin LA. Lower practice effects as a marker of cognitive performance and dementia risk: A literature review. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2020;12(1):2055.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper DBP, Lacritz LH, Weiner MF, Rosenberg RN, Cullum CM. Category fluency in mild cognitive impairment: reduced effect of practice in test-retest conditions. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004;18(3):120–2.PubMedCrossRef Cooper DBP, Lacritz LH, Weiner MF, Rosenberg RN, Cullum CM. Category fluency in mild cognitive impairment: reduced effect of practice in test-retest conditions. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004;18(3):120–2.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Darby D, Maruff P, Collie A, McStephen M. Mild cognitive impairment can be detected by multiple assessments in a single day. Neurology. 2002;59(7):1042–6.PubMedCrossRef Darby D, Maruff P, Collie A, McStephen M. Mild cognitive impairment can be detected by multiple assessments in a single day. Neurology. 2002;59(7):1042–6.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Duff K, Beglinger LJ, Van Der Heiden S, et al. Short-term practice effects in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: implications for diagnosis and treatment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20(5):986–99.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Duff K, Beglinger LJ, Van Der Heiden S, et al. Short-term practice effects in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: implications for diagnosis and treatment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20(5):986–99.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Duff K, Chelune G, Dennett K. Within-session practice effects in patients referred for suspected dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2012;33(4):245–9.PubMedCrossRef Duff K, Chelune G, Dennett K. Within-session practice effects in patients referred for suspected dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2012;33(4):245–9.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Kito Y, Kazui H, Kubo Y, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Behav Neurol. 2009;21(3):165–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kito Y, Kazui H, Kubo Y, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Behav Neurol. 2009;21(3):165–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Lindqvist G, Malmgren H. Organic mental disorders as hypothetical pathogenetic processes. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1993;373:5–17.PubMedCrossRef Lindqvist G, Malmgren H. Organic mental disorders as hypothetical pathogenetic processes. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1993;373:5–17.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Klinge PM, Brooks DJ, Samii A, et al. Correlates of local cerebral blood flow (CBF) in normal pressure hydrocephalus patients before and after shunting–A retrospective analysis of [(15)O]H(2)O PET-CBF studies in 65 patients. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110(4):369–77.PubMedCrossRef Klinge PM, Brooks DJ, Samii A, et al. Correlates of local cerebral blood flow (CBF) in normal pressure hydrocephalus patients before and after shunting–A retrospective analysis of [(15)O]H(2)O PET-CBF studies in 65 patients. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110(4):369–77.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Serial Tap Test of patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus: impact on cognitive function and its meaning
verfasst von
Samanta Fabrício Blattes da Rocha
Pedro André Kowacs
Ricardo Krause Martinez de Souza
Matheus Kahakura Franco Pedro
Ricardo Ramina
Hélio A. Ghizoni Teive
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Fluids and Barriers of the CNS / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 2045-8118
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12987-021-00254-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

Fluids and Barriers of the CNS 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Nicht Creutzfeldt Jakob, sondern Abführtee-Vergiftung

29.05.2024 Hyponatriämie Nachrichten

Eine ältere Frau trinkt regelmäßig Sennesblättertee gegen ihre Verstopfung. Der scheint plötzlich gut zu wirken. Auf Durchfall und Erbrechen folgt allerdings eine Hyponatriämie. Nach deren Korrektur kommt es plötzlich zu progredienten Kognitions- und Verhaltensstörungen.

Schutz der Synapsen bei Alzheimer

29.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Mit einem Neurotrophin-Rezeptor-Modulator lässt sich möglicherweise eine bestehende Alzheimerdemenz etwas abschwächen: Erste Phase-2-Daten deuten auf einen verbesserten Synapsenschutz.

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.